Benham's Top
posted on 3 Dec 2013 by guy
last changed 14 Jun 2014
Your vote (click to rate) ages: 5 to 99 yrs budget: $0.00 to $2.00 prep time: 0 to 30 min class time: 10 to 60 min |
In 1894, Charles Benham invented a top with a black and white pattern that produces a sense of color when it spins. This lesson contains instructions and materials for producing your own Benham top, as well as a short explanation behind the false color perception. Although the scientific explanation of Benham's top can be a challenge for 13-year-olds (as it is for professional researchers in the subject), even 5-year-olds can enjoy making the top and viewing the colors. |
subjects: Biology, Psychology
keywords: Benham, top, disk, color perception, retina
Fig. 1: Benham's Disk. Named after Charles Benham, who sold toy tops with designs like this one painted on them. Click on the thumbnail image to jump to an animation of Benham's disk by Michael Bach at Freiburg. Requires Flash Player.
In 1894, Charles Benham designed a false color illusion, which he marketed as a toy top painted with the pattern shown in figure 1. As the black and white pattern rotates, most people perceive faint colors from the narrow stripes.
activity — Benham's Disk
You can make your own version of Benham's disk by printing out the attached file and gluing or taping it to an old compact disk that you no longer need. Take a size 1 rubber stopper with a hole in the middle (or a similar sized cork that you drill), put the stopper in the CD and stick a short pencil through the stopper. If you are drilling a cork, you may choose to leave the drill bit in the cork instead of a pencil. Now you have a top you can spin. Try it at different speeds, both clockwise and counter-clockwise, and under different illuminations, to see different colors.
As the pattern in figure 1 is rotated, most people can see the four sets of stripes take on different pale colors. If the rotation is clockwise, the most typical colors are (from innermost ring to outermost)
- red or red-brown,
- pink or green,
- pale blue,
- dark blue or violet.
When the rotation reverses, so does the pattern of apparent colors: the innermost ring becomes dark blue and the outermost becomes red. The colors vary noticably with illumination and rotation speed, but under most normal daylight conditions, the most saturated colors are achieved with a rotation speed in the range of 5 to 10 revolutions per second.
Michael Bach at Freiburg has made a very elegant animation of Benham's top, which you can go to by clicking the thumbnail image of figure 1. (Be sure to also check out his full collection of fabulous optical illusions.)
what's going on?
Early efforts to explain the appearance of Benham's colors incorrectly suggested that the color arises from variable latency in the different types of color cones of the retina. ("Latency" refers to the lag time it takes for a visual signal to be produced after light strikes the retina.) Some modern texts continue to propagate this mistaken idea.
According to the latency explanation, the three different types of color cones respond at different rates. When light hits the retina, some cones respond faster than others (red sensitive cones are believed to respond faster than blue or green cones). The first type of cone to respond sends its color signal to the brain before the other cones catch up, thereby giving the impression of a colored image.
It's fairly easy to discredit this explanation of Benham's colors. If a difference in cone latency were the full explanation, the perceived colors should depend only on the frequency of changes between light and dark. In particular, they should not depend on the direction of rotation; at any given radius, the fraction of time the region is white and the fraction of time it is black is the same no matter which direction the top is rotating. The fact that the colors change when the rotation is reversed suggests the effect must depend on more than just the time spent in white and black regions.
Research in the 1990's1 suggested that Benham colors depend on the changes between black and white stripes relative to changes of the background (as well as the frequency of both). In one direction, the stripe will change from black to white shortly after the background changes from black to white, and in the other direction the stripe will change a longer time after the background changes. As the rotation reverses, this observation suggests that the colors should swap between inner and outer rings, as they appear to do.
Observations also indicate that the area surrounding the stripes plays an important role. If the stripes are very broad, the apparent colors are restricted to the inner and outer edges of the stripes. Only thin stripes give a complete impression of color.
Fig. 2: Neural connections in the retina. Horizontal cells (green) and amacrine cells (grey) make lateral connections across different regions of the retina.
This behavior suggests that Benham colors originate from comparisons of one visual region to another, most likely from lateral connections in the retina. As figure 2 indicates, there are horizontal and amacrine nerve cells in the retina that make lateral connections between one area of the retina to another, and these connections may provide the comparative information the brain needs to produce Benham colors.
teaching notes
For an audience with a long enough attention span, try some variations in the Benham experiment. To best record the colors that people see, ask them to match colors to a set of color chips (see under "further resources" for a source of color chips).
Suggested variations:
- Reverse direction of the top rotation.
- Change the rotation speed.
- Change the intensity of illumination.
- Use monochromatic light (christmas tree lights work well for this experiment).
- Use a pattern with broader stripes, or bring the eye closer to the top so that the stripes take up a larger field of view. As the stripes get broader, the colors should be restricted to the edges of the stripes.
- Look through a cardboard paper towel tube at a portion of the spinning top. Research indicates that if the surrounding field of view is restricted, Benham's colors can be made to disappear.
further resources
Accurate color comparisons between different observers is best done with a set of standard color charts. You can use the popular Munsell system of color chips or the RGB or CYMK systems popular with computer displays or printers. A good set of RGB colors is available at http://www.rapidtables.com/web/color/RGB_Color.htm.
- 1. von Campenhausen, Christoph and Schramme, Jürgen. "100 years of Benham's top in colour science" Perception 24(6) 1995: 695-717.